Aciman, A. (2000). Shadow cities.
In A. Aciman (Ed.), Letters of transit:
Reflections on exile, identity, language and loss (pp. 15-34). New York : The New Press.
Through a discussion of his
favorite park on the Upper West Side of Manhattan, Aciman – a self-described “exile”
from Alexandria in Egypt – argues for the existence of what he calls “shadow
cities,” effectively versions of the remembrances of other cities one has
inhabited for any length of time that exist alongside, underneath and through
one’s current locale. These shadow cities allow us to appreciate where we are
more fully by creating a mirror, which Aciman calls a “mnemonic correlate,” in
which one can see these other cities one has known or even only imagined
inhabiting. For instance, he says, he never actually sees the real New York ; instead, he only sees a New York which stands in for other locales
or helps him to remember the other places he has been. Exiles, he argues, fear
change and instinctively look for their homeland abroad in order to minimize
the stress of the longing and loss they feel. This is, effectively, the opposite
of the concept that you can never go home again – you’re always home wherever
you are, because you are constantly bringing your concept of “home” with you,
wherever you go.
Chabon, M. (2009, 16 July). Manhood for amateurs: The wilderness of
childhood. The New York
Review of Books, 56, 12.
Chabon reminisces about his
childhood home in Maryland , where his family
lived at the edge of a wooded area. He connects the “wilderness of childhood”
to well-known children’s stories of adventure and their usual inclusion of a map
which not only reveals the geography of the tale but also serves as metaphor
for the mental maps which children create of their surroundings as they learn
and grow. Recalling his interest in a series of books of the lives of famous
Americans as children, Chabon finds a familiar link from their adventures in
the wilderness through his play (and mental map-making) as a child and
contrasts that with the current generation of American children, including his
own, who don’t get to experience the wilderness of childhood for fear of
abduction or otherwise serious harm. Ultimately, he worries what effect this
closing of the wilderness will have on both children’s imaginations – their
ability to play, think and create freely, unencumbered by adults – and “the
world of adventure, of stories, of literature itself.”
Cooper, C. (1974). The house as symbol of the self. In J.
Lang, et al. (Eds.), Designing for Human
Behavior (pp. 130-146). Stroudsburg , PA :
Dowden, Hutchinson and Ross.
In this “think piece,” Cooper takes the Jungian concepts of
the collective unconscious, archetypes and symbols and relates them to the
modern construct of the house. For Jung, archetypes are nodes of psychic energy
in the unconscious mind, and symbols are the manifestations of archetypes in
the real world. For Cooper, the house serves as a symbol of the self, and
humans treat their homes as reflections of the people they are, the people they
want others to see them as, or the people they someday hope to be. This
house-as-self image that individuals project may often happen unconsciously,
but it is fraught with the same stereotypes and judgments that are found in issues
of socioeconomic status, racial inequality and cultural normalcy. She then
examines examples of house-as-self in literature and poetry, finding that the
house as “womb” or “mother” is a common theme. Further connecting to research
of Carl Jung, Cooper uses examples of Jung’s accounts of his own dreams of
houses and subsequent actual additions to his house to show that the collective
unconscious is constantly at work building and shaping our homes and, by
extension, our lives. Although somewhat dated, Cooper’s collection of research
and anecdotal evidence ultimately suggests that architects and designers need
to consider their clients’ sense of self when designing for them or risk producing
“a symbolic reality which leaves the residents bewildered and resentful.”
Proshansky, H. M., Fabian, A. K. & Kaminoff, R. (1983).
Place-identity: Physical world socialization of the self. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 3(1), pp. 57-83.
The authors examine the concept of “place-identity,” whereby
they extrapolate from the development of self-identity a concurrent theory
concerning an individual’s learned ability to make distinctions between oneself
and not only objects and things but also the spaces and places in which those
things are found. This place-identity is crucial to the development of one’s
ability to effectively respond to a variety of physical settings encountered
throughout life, they argue, whether or not one has been exposed to a
particular environment before. In this way, we collect a database of
place-identities which allow us to navigate our way through life. They discuss
the various functions of place-identity, including recognition, meaning, expressive-requirement,
mediating change, and anxiety and defense. Physical settings dominate the lives
of children, and the authors insist that it is within the framework of these
spaces where children learn the significant social roles they will inhabit for
the rest of their lives. Therefore, place-identity is instrumental in the
formation of one’s self-identity and its importance cannot be overstated. Their
exhaustive analysis of place-identity is largely effective, though it often
diverges into somewhat convoluted and repetitive reasoning which tend to lessen
the impact of the basic argument. Plainer language and a tighter thematic framework
could strengthen their discussion considerably.
Whitehead, C. (2001, 11 November). The way we live now:
11-11-01; Lost and found. The New York
Times. Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com
In an article written two months after the terrorist
attacks of September 11, Whitehead rather effectively argues, through humor no
less, that you can call yourself a New Yorker “when what was there before is
more real and solid than what is here now” – or, rather, once you’ve created
your own private version of the city which is based on all of the places you’ve
lived, frequented, worked or otherwise known. The places that have disappeared
still exist, he says, because we still exist and we still remember. Not only
that, but having this history also proves that you were part of the city and,
by extension, it was a part of you. It’s a rather poignant reaction to the
destruction of the two World Trade Center towers, which Whitehead
suggests still stand “because we saw them…were lucky enough to know them for a
time.” He ultimately wonders what, if anything, will physically take their
place and suggests that we give whatever does a chance to share the city with
us too.
Wow! now i realize how different our interpretations of the readings could be! Not only from the discussions in class, but from reading our annotated bibliography's - lets me see another way of looking at the readings!
ReplyDeleteWas there one in particular, or were all of them different? Nora is definitely right when she says there isn't only one way to understand them, though I'd like to think there are some basic tenets we can all pretty much agree on.
ReplyDeleteI do think my Whitehead reading was a bit off, actually, now that I look back at it. "Humor" isn't really the right word; though parts of it made me laugh, it was more like "wry observations" or something of the sort.
I really enjoyed the writing you did here. I agree that the Proshansky piece would be more effective if the language were simpler. I wonder if it was complicated by having three authors!
ReplyDelete-Larry